Serial killers, spiders and cybersex: Social and survival information bias in the transmission of urban legends
Corresponding Author
Joseph M. Stubbersfield
Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, UK
Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, School of Education, Durham University, UK
Correspondence should be addressed to Joseph M. Stubbersfield, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, Dawson Building, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK (email: [email protected]).Search for more papers by this authorJamshid J. Tehrani
Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, UK
Search for more papers by this authorEmma G. Flynn
Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, School of Education, Durham University, UK
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Joseph M. Stubbersfield
Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, UK
Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, School of Education, Durham University, UK
Correspondence should be addressed to Joseph M. Stubbersfield, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, Dawson Building, South Road, Durham DH1 3LE, UK (email: [email protected]).Search for more papers by this authorJamshid J. Tehrani
Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, UK
Search for more papers by this authorEmma G. Flynn
Centre for the Coevolution of Biology and Culture, School of Education, Durham University, UK
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
This study uses urban legends to examine the effects of the social information bias and survival information bias on cultural transmission across three phases of transmission: the choose-to-receive phase, the encode-and-retrieve phase, and the choose-to-transmit phase. In line with previous research into content biases, a linear transmission chain design with 60 participants aged 18–52 was used to examine the encode-and-retrieve phase, while participants were asked to rank their interest in reading the story behind a headline and passing a story on for the other two phases. Legends which contained social information (Social Type), legends which contained survival information (Survival Type), and legends which contained both forms of information (Combined Type) were all recalled with significantly greater accuracy than control material, while Social and Combined Type legends were recalled with significantly greater accuracy than Survival Type legends. In another study with 30 participants aged 18–22, no significant differences were found between legend types in either the choose-to-receive phase or the choose-to-transmit phase.
Supporting Information
Filename | Description |
---|---|
bjop12073-sup-0001-DataS1-S7.docxWord document, 49.6 KB | Data S1. Questionnaire used in Study 1. Data S2. Table showing legends' mean scores from Study 1. Data S3. Tables showing homogeneous subsets and means. Data S4. Full text of urban legends used in Studies 2 and 3. Data S5. Table showing mean differences between the legends used in Study 2. Data S6. Tables showing comparisons between the models used in the analysis of Study 2. Data S7. Full equation for the model used in the analyses in Study 2. |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
References
- Bangerter, A. (2000). Transformation between scientific and social representations of conception: The method of serial reproduction. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 405–423. doi:10.1348/0144666042565353
10.1348/0144666042565353 Google Scholar
- Barrett, J. L., & Nyhof, M. A. (2001). Spreading of non-natural concepts: The role if intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 69–100. doi:10.1163/156853701300063589
10.1163/156853701300063589 Google Scholar
- Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Oxford, UK: Macmillan.
- Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). l me4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Eigen and S4 (R package Version 1.0-6) [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
- Best, J., & Horiuchi, G. T. (1985). The razor blade in the apple: The social construction of urban legends. Social Problems, 32, 488–499. doi:10.1525/sp.1985.32.5.03a00070
- Brunvand, J. H. (1986). The Mexican pet. New York: Norton.
- Brunvand, J. H. (1999). Too good to be true: The colossal book of urban legends. New York: Norton.
- Brunvand, J. H. (2000). The truth never stands in the way of a good story. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.
- Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (Eds.) (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
- Clutton-Brock, T. H., & Harvey, P. H. (1980). Primates, brains and ecology. Journal of the Zoological Society of London, 190, 309–323. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1980.tb01430.x
10.1111/j.1469-7998.1980.tb01430.x Google Scholar
- Dégh, L., & Vazsonyi, A. (1975). The hypothesis of multi-conduit transmission in folklore. In D. Ben-Amos & K. S. Goldstein (Eds.), Folklore: Performance and communication (pp. 207–252). Paris, France: Mouton.
- Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 16, 681–735. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00032325
- Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178–190. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-8
- Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–181. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.093158
- Ellis, B. (1983). De legendis urbis: Modern legends in ancient Rome. Journal of American Folklore, 96, 200–208. doi:10.2307/540293
- Eriksson, K., & Coultas, J. C. (2014). Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 14, 1–26. doi:10.1163/15685373-12342107
- Fine, G. A. (1979). Cokelore and Coke law: Urban belief tales and the problem of multiple origins. Journal of American Folklore, 92, 477–482. doi:10.2307/540512
- Fine, G. A. (1980). The Kentucky fried rat: Legends and modern society. Journal of the Folklore Institute, 17, 222–243. doi:10.2307/3813896
- Fox Tree, J. E., & Weldon, M. S. (2007). Retelling urban legends. The American Journal of Psychology, 120, 459–476. doi:10.2307/20445414
- Heath, C., Bell, C., & Sternberg, E. (2001). Emotional selection in memes: The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1028–1041. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1028
- Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous Inference in General Parametric Models. Biometrical Journal, 50, 346–363. doi:10.1002/bimj.200810425
- Kang, S., McDermott, K. B., & Cohen, S. (2008). The mnemonic advantage of processing fitness-relevant information. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1151–1156. doi:10.3758/MC.36.6.1151
- Kashima, Y. (1997). Culture, narrative, and human motivation. In D. Munro, J. F. Schumaker & S. C. Carr (Eds.), Motivation and culture (pp. 16–30). New York: Routledge.
- Kashima, Y. (2000). Maintaining cultural stereotypes in the serial reproduction of narratives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 594–604. doi:10.1177/0146167200267007
- Kashima, Y., Lyons, A., & Clark, A. (2013). The maintenance of cultural stereotypes in the conversational retelling of narratives. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 60–70. doi:10.1111/ajsp.12004
- Kintsch, W. (1974). The representation of meaning in memory. Oxford, UK: Erlbaum.
- Lyons, A., & Kashima, Y. (2003). How are stereotypes maintained through communication? The influence of stereotype sharedness. Journal of personality and social psychology, 85(6), 989. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.989
- Lyons, A., & Kashima, Y. (2006). Maintaining stereotypes in communication: Investigating memory biases and coherence-seeking in storytelling. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 9, 59–71. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2006.00184.x
- Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 173–192. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00073.x
- McGuigan, N., & Cubillo, M. (2013). Information transmission in young children: When social information is more important than nonsocial information. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174, 605–619. doi:10.1080/00221325.2012.749833
- Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2004). The hierarchical transformation of event knowledge in human cultural transmission. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 1–24. doi:10.1163/156853704323074732
10.1163/156853704323074732 Google Scholar
- Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3489–3501. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0129
- Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 405–423. doi:10.1348/000712605X85871
- Mullen, P. B. (1972). Modern legend and rumor theory. Journal of the Folklore Institute, 9, 95–109. doi:10.2307/3814160
10.2307/3814160 Google Scholar
- Nairne, J. S. (2010). Adaptive memory: Evolutionary constraints on remembering. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 53, 1–32. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(10)53001-9
- Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2008). Adaptive memory: Is survival processing special? Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 377–385. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.001
- Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2010). Adaptive memory: Nature's criterion and the functionalist agenda. American Journal of Psychology, 4, 381–390. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0381
10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0381 Google Scholar
- Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Thompson, S. R. (2008). Adaptive memory: The comparative value of survival processing. Psychological Science, 19, 176–180. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02064.x
- Nairne, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2007). Adaptive memory: Survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 263–273. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.263
- Nielson, M., Cucchiaro, J., & Mohamedally, J. (2012). When the transmission of culture is child's play. PLoS ONE, 7, e34066. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034066
- Otgaar, H., Smeets, T., & van Bergen, S. (2010). Picturing survival memories: Enhanced memory after fitness-relevant processing occurs for verbal and visual stimuli. Memory and Cognition, 38, 23–28. doi:10.3758/MC.38.1.23
- Owens, J., Bower, G. H., & Black, J. B. (1979). The soap opera effect in story recall. Memory and Cognition, 7, 185–191. doi:10.3758/BF03197537
- Packman, J. L., & Battig, W. F. (1978). Effects of different kinds of semantic processing on memory for words. Memory and Cognition, 6, 502–508. doi:10.3758/BF03198238
- R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 3.0.2) [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/
- Reysen, M. B., Talbert, N. G., Dominko, M., Jones, A. N., & Kelley, M. R. (2011). The effects of collaboration on recall of social information. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 646–661. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02035.x
- Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and counting-out rhymes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press on Demand.
10.1093/oso/9780195082111.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Shibutani, T. (1966). Improvised News: A Sociological Study of Rumor. New York: Bobbs-Merrill.
- Simpson, J. (1981). Rationalized motifs in urban legends. Folklore, 92, 203–207. doi:10.1080/0015587X.1981.9716207
- Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Stubbersfield, J. M., Tehrani, J. J., & Flynn, E. G. (2014). Santa crucified and death by lollypop: Culture, cognition and the epidemiology of urban legends. Manuscript submitted for publication.
- Sugiyama, M. S. (2001). Food, foragers, and folklore: The role of narrative in human subsistence. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 22, 221–240. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00063-0
- Tangherlini, T. R. (1990). “It happened not too far from here..”: A survey of legend theory and characterization. Western Folklore, 49, 371–390. doi:10.2307/1499751
- Victor, J. (1990). Satanic cult rumors as contemporary legend. Western Folklore, 49, 51–81. doi:10.2307/1499482
- Von Sydow, C. W. (1965). Folktale studies and philology: Some points of view. In A. Dundes (Ed.), The study of folklore (pp. 219–242). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. (Original work published 1948).
- Weinstein, Y., Bugg, J. M., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). Can the survival recall advantage be explained by basic memory processes? Memory and Cognition, 36, 913–919. doi:10.3758/MC.36.5.913
- Whiten, A. (1999). Machiavellian intelligence hypothesis. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 495–497). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Whiten , A. , Byrne & R. W. (Eds.) (1997). Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations (Vol. 2). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University.