How do we process prices? Electrophysiological evidence of componential analysis
Fernando Ojedo
Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Spain
Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Center (CIMCYC), Granada, Spain
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Pedro Macizo
Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Spain
Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Center (CIMCYC), Granada, Spain
Correspondence should be addressed to Pedro Macizo, Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Campus de Cartuja, s/n. 18071, Granada, Spain (email: [email protected]).Search for more papers by this authorFernando Ojedo
Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Spain
Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Center (CIMCYC), Granada, Spain
Search for more papers by this authorCorresponding Author
Pedro Macizo
Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Spain
Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Center (CIMCYC), Granada, Spain
Correspondence should be addressed to Pedro Macizo, Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Campus de Cartuja, s/n. 18071, Granada, Spain (email: [email protected]).Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
The aim of this study was to evaluate if the processing of prices (e.g., 8 euro) involves separate analysis of the digit (8) and the monetary category (euro). Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded when participants performed a price comparison task in which a pair of prices was presented and they selected the one with higher monetary value. There were compatible comparisons where the digit and the monetary category of one price were higher than those of the other price (e.g., 7 euro – 4 cent, 7 > 4 and euro > cent), and incompatible comparisons where the number of one price was larger but the monetary category smaller than those of the other price (e.g., 4 euro – 7 cent, 4 < 7 but euro > cent). Compatibility effect modulated ERPs in the 350–450 ms time window, with more negative amplitudes in incompatible trials relative to compatible trials. This pattern of results suggests that prices were processed in a componential manner, challenging the evidences of the holistic model. The results of this study agree with the general model framework for multi-symbol number comparison: The constituents of a multi-symbol magnitude are processed and compared to each other separately in a componential manner.
Supporting Information
Filename | Description |
---|---|
bjop12425-sup-0001-DataS1.docxWord document, 65.7 KB | Data S1. ERP analysis of the magnitude of prices. |
bjop12425-sup-0002-DataS2.docxWord document, 282.4 KB | Data S2. Grand average ERPs obtained in the study. |
Please note: The publisher is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting information supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing content) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.
References
- Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57, 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
- Cao, B., Gao, H., & Li, F. (2015). Hybrid model of price pair comparisons: Evidence from an event-related potential study. NeuroReport, 26, 838–841. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000433
- Cao, B., Li, H., Li, F., Zhang, L., & Wang, Y. (2012). The holistic processing of price comparison: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidences. Biological Psychology, 89(1), 63–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.005
- Dehaene, S. (1996). The organization of brain activations in number comparison: Event-related potentials and the additive-factors method. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(1), 47–68. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1996.8.1.47
- Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 626–641. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.626
- Dehaene, S., & Marques, J. F. (2002). Cognitive euroscience: Scalar variability in price estimation and the cognitive consequences of switching to the euro. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 55, 705–731. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980244000044
- Goldman, R., Ganor-Stern, D., & Tzelgov, T. (2012). “On the money”. Monetary and numerical judgments of currency. Acta Psychologica, 141(2), 222–230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.07.005
- Greenhouse, S., & Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika, 24, 95–112. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02289823
- Hinrichs, J. V., Yurko, D. S., & Hu, J. M. (1981). Two-digit number comparison: Use of place information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 890–901. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.7.4.890
- Huber, S., Bahnmueller, J., Klein, E., & Moeller, K. (2015). Testing a model of componential processing of multi-symbol numbers-evidence from measurement units. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 1417–1423. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0805-8
- Huber, S., Cornelesen, S., Moeller, K., & Nuerk, H. C. (2015). Towards a model framework of generalized parallel componential processing of multi-symbol numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition., 41, 732–745. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000043
- Huber, S., Klein, E., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Moeller, K. (2014). Decimal representations are not distinct from natural number representations—evidence from a combined eye-tracking and computational modelling approach. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 172. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00172
- Huber, S., Moeller, K., Nuerk, H. C., & Willmes, K. (2013). A computational modelling approach on three-digit number processing. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5, 317–334. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12016
- Huber, S., Nuerk, H. C., Willmes, K., & Moeller, K. (2016). A general model framework for multisymbol number comparison. Psychological Review, 123(6), 667. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000040
- Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203–205. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7350657
- Libertus, M. E., Woldorff, M. G., & Brannon, E. M. (2007). Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-3-1
- Liotti, M., Woldorff, M. G., Perez, R., & Mayberg, H. S. (2000). An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect. Neuropsychologia, 38, 701–711. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00106-2
- Macizo, P. (2017). Conflict resolution in two-digit number processing: Evidence of an inhibitory mechanism. Psychological Research, 81(1), 219–230. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0716-3
- Macizo, P., & Herrera, A. (2010). Two-digit number comparison: Decade-unit and unit-decade produce the same compatibility effect with number words. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 64(1), 17–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015803
- Macizo, P., & Herrera, A. (2011). Cognitive control in number processing: Evidence from the unit-decade compatibility effect. Acta Psychologica, 136(1), 112–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.10.008
- Macizo, P., & Herrera, A. (2013). Do people access meaning when they name banknotes? Cognitive Processing, 14(1), 43–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0531-3
- Macizo, P., Herrera, A., Paolieri, D., & Román, P. (2010). Is there cross-language modulation when bilinguals process number words? Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 651–669. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000184
- Macizo, P., Herrera, A., Román, P., & Martín, M. C. (2011a). The processing of two-digit numbers in bilinguals. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 464–477. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02005
- Macizo, P., Herrera, A., Román, P., & Martín, M. C. (2011b). Proficiency in a second language influences the processing of number words. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 915–921. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2011.586626
- Macizo, P., & Morales, L. (2015). Cognitive processing of currency: Euros and dollars. British Journal of Experimental Psychology, 106(4), 583–596. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12114
- Macizo, P., & Ojedo, F. (2017). The processing of price comparison is not holistic but componential. Psychological Research, 82, 1144–1157. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0884-4
- Moeller, K., Huber, S., Nuerk, H. C., & Willmes, K. (2011). Two-digit number processing: Holistic, decomposed or hybrid? A computational modelling approach Psychological Research, 75, 290–306. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0307-2
- Moeller, K., Klein, E., Nuerk, H. C., & Willmes, K. (2013). Magnitude representation in sequential comparison of two-digit numbers is not holistic either. Cognitive Processing, 14(1), 51–62. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0535-z
- Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520. https://doi.org/10.1038/2151519a0
- Nuerk, H. C., Moeller, K., Klein, E., Willmes, K., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Extending the mental number line – A review of multi-digit number processing. Journal of Psychology, 219(1), 3–22. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000041
- Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82(1), B25–B33. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00142-1
- Nuerk, H. C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2004). On the perceptual generality of the unit-decade compatibility effect. Experimental Psychology, 51(1), 72–79. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.51.1.72
- Nuerk, H. C., & Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers. Psychology Sciences, 47(1), 52–72.
- Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Rivière, D., & LeBihan, D. (2001). Modulation of parietal activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task. NeuroImage, 14, 1013–1026. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0913
- Rebai, M., Bernard, C., & Lannou, J. (1997). The Stroop's test evokes a negative brain potential, the N400. International Journal of Neuroscience, 91(1–2), 85–94. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459708986367
- Ruiz, A., Togato, G., García-Gámez, A. B., & Macizo, P. (2017). Individual differences in banknote processing: The role of altruism. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(2), 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1237954
- Schwarz, W., & Heinze, H. J. (1998). On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: A behavioral and event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 36(11), 1167–1179. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00001-3
- Szűcs, D., & Soltész, F. (2007). Event-related potentials dissociate facilitation and interference effects in the numerical Stroop paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3190–3202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.013
- Szűcs, D., & Soltész, F. (2010). Event-related brain potentials to violations of arithmetic syntax represented by place value structure. Biological Psychology, 84(2), 354–367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.04.002
- Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2001). Using multivariate statistics ( 4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
- Temple, E., & Posner, M. (1998). Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 7836–7841. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.13.7836
- Thomas, M., & Morwitz, V. G. (2005). Penny wise and pound foolish: The left digit effect in price cognition. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(1), 54–64. https://doi.org/10.1086/429600
- Turconi, E., Jemel, B., Rossion, B., & Seron, X. (2004). Electrophysiological evidence for differential processing of numerical quantity and order in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(1), 22–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.05.003
- West, R., & Alain, C. (1999). Event-related neural activity associated with the Stroop task. Cognitive Brain Research, 8(2), 157–164. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00017-8
- West, R., Jakubek, K., Wymbs, N., Perry, M., & Moore, K. (2005). Neural correlates of conflict processing. Experimental Brain Research, 167(1), 38–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-2366-y
- Wood, G., Nuerk, H. C., & Willmes, K. (2006). Neural representations of two-digit numbers: A parametric fMRI study. NeuroImage, 29(2), 358–367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.056